Katana VentraIP

Negative affectivity

Negative affectivity (NA), or negative affect, is a personality variable that involves the experience of negative emotions and poor self-concept.[1] Negative affectivity subsumes a variety of negative emotions, including anger, contempt, disgust, guilt, fear,[2] and nervousness. Low negative affectivity is characterized by frequent states of calmness and serenity, along with states of confidence, activeness, and great enthusiasm.

Individuals differ in negative emotional reactivity.[3] Trait negative affectivity roughly corresponds to the dominant personality factor of anxiety/neuroticism that is found within the Big Five personality traits as emotional stability.[4] The Big Five are characterized as openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism. Neuroticism can plague an individual with severe mood swings, frequent sadness, worry, and being easily disturbed,[1][3] and predicts the development and onset of all "common" mental disorders.[5] Research shows that negative affectivity relates to different classes of variables: Self-reported stress and (poor) coping skills,[1][6][7] health complaints, and frequency of unpleasant events.[8] Weight gain and mental health complaints are often experienced as well.


People who express high negative affectivity view themselves and a variety of aspects of the world around them in generally negative terms.[1] Negative affectivity is strongly related to life satisfaction. Individuals high in negative affect will exhibit, on average, higher levels of distress, anxiety, and dissatisfaction, and tend to focus on the unpleasant aspects of themselves, the world, the future, and other people, and also evoke more negative life events.[8] The similarities between these affective traits and life satisfaction have led some researchers to view both positive and negative affect with life satisfaction as specific indicators of the broader construct of subjective well-being.


Negative affect arousal mechanisms can induce negative affective states as evidenced by a study conducted by Stanley S. Seidner on negative arousal and white noise. The study quantified reactions from Mexican and Puerto Rican participants in response to the devaluation of speakers from other ethnic origins.[9]

Measurement[edit]

There are many instruments that can be used to measure negative affectivity, including measures of related concepts, such as neuroticism and trait anxiety.[10] Two frequently used are:


PANAS The Positive and Negative Affect Schedule incorporates a 10-item negative affect scale.[11] The PANAS-X is an expanded version of PANAS that incorporates negative affect subscales for Fear, Sadness, Guilt, Hostility, and Shyness.


I-PANAS-SF – The International Positive and Negative Affect Schedule Short Form is an extensively validated brief, cross-culturally reliable 10-item version of the PANAS.[12] Negative Affect items are Afraid, Ashamed, Hostile, Nervous and Upset. Internal consistency reliabilities between .72 and .76 are reported. The I-PANAS-SF was developed to eliminate redundant and ambiguous items and thereby derive an efficient measure for general use in research situations where either time or space are limited, or where international populations are of interest but where English may not be the mother tongue.[12]

Affect (psychology)

Emotion

Health psychology

Personality psychology

Positive affectivity

Toxic positivity

Beiser, M (1974). "Components and correlates of mental well-being". Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 15 (4): 320–327. :10.2307/2137092. JSTOR 2137092. PMID 4455735.

doi

Bradburn, N. M. (1969). The structure of psychological well-being. Chicago: Aldine.

DeNeve, K. M.; Cooper, H. (1998). "The happy personality: a meta-analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being". Psychological Bulletin. 124 (2): 197–229. :10.1037/0033-2909.124.2.197. PMID 9747186.

doi

Jex, S. M.; Spector, P. E. (1996). "The impact of negative affectivity on stressor strain relations: A replication and extension". Work and Stress. 10: 36–45. :10.1080/02678379608256783.

doi

Judge, T. A.; Locke, E. A.; Durham, C. C.; Kluger, A. N. (1998). "Dispositional effects on job and life satisfaction: the role of core evaluations". Journal of Applied Psychology. 83 (1): 17–34. :10.1037/0021-9010.83.1.17. PMID 9494439.

doi

Kanner, A. D.; Coyne, J. C.; Schaefer, C.; Lazarus, R. S. (1981). "). Comparison of two modes of stress measurement: Daily hassles and uplifts versus major life events". Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 4 (1): 1–39. :10.1007/bf00844845. PMID 7288876. S2CID 13229341.

doi

Larsen, R. J.; Ketelaar, T. (1989). (PDF). Personality and Individual Differences. 10 (12): 1221–1228. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(89)90233-x. hdl:2027.42/28239.

"Extraversion, neuroticism, and susceptibility to positive and negative mood induction procedures"

Norton, P. (2011). Risk Factors in the Development of Anxiety Disorders: Negative Affectivity. "Strides", 2–5.

Pressman, S. & Cohen, S. (2006). Positive Affect and Health. "Current Directions in Psychological Science, Carnegie Mellon University" 122–125.

Sheppard, L., Krammer, G., & Bodenhausen, G. (1994). Negative affect and social judgment: the differential impact of anger and sadness. "European Journal of Social Psychology", 46–51.

Stone, A. A. (1981). "The association between perceptions of daily experiences and self- and spouse-rated mood". Journal of Research in Personality. 15 (4): 510–522. :10.1016/0092-6566(81)90047-7.

doi