Katana VentraIP

Reading comprehension

Reading comprehension is the ability to process written text, understand its meaning, and to integrate with what the reader already knows.[1][2][3][4] Reading comprehension relies on two abilities that are connected to each other: word reading and language comprehension.[5] Comprehension specifically is a "creative, multifaceted process" that is dependent upon four language skills: phonology, syntax, semantics, and pragmatics.[6]

This article is about human reading comprehension. For machine reading comprehension, see natural-language understanding. For the article on reading, see Reading.

Some of the fundamental skills required in efficient reading comprehension are the ability to:[7][8][9]


Comprehension skills that can be applied as well as taught to all reading situations include:[10]


There are many reading strategies to use in improving reading comprehension and inferences, these include improving one's vocabulary, critical text analysis (intertextuality, actual events vs. narration of events, etc.), and practising deep reading.[11] The ability to comprehend text is influenced by the readers' skills and their ability to process information. If word recognition is difficult, students tend to use too much of their processing capacity to read individual words which interferes with their ability to comprehend what is read.

Provides a model of fluent reading and helps students learn decoding skills by offering positive feedback.

[36]

Provides direct opportunities for a teacher to circulate in the class, observe students, and offer individual remediation.

[37]

Difficult or complex content[edit]

Reading difficult texts[edit]

Some texts, like in philosophy, literature or scientific research, may appear more difficult to read because of the prior knowledge they assume, the tradition from which they come, or the tone, such as criticizing or parodying. A Philosopher Jacques Derrida, explained his opinion about complicated text: "In order to unfold what is implicit in so many discourses, one would have each time to make a pedagogical outlay that is just not reasonable to expect from every book. Here the responsibility has to be shared out, mediated; the reading has to do its work and the work has to make its reader."[48] Other Philosophers however, believe that if you have something to say, you should be able to make the message readable to a wide audience.[49]

Hyperlinks[edit]

Embedded hyperlinks in documents or Internet pages have been found to make different demands on the reader than traditional text. Authors such as Nicholas Carr, and Psychologists, such as Maryanne Wolf, contend that the internet may have a negative impact on attention and reading comprehension.[50] Some studies report increased demands of reading hyperlinked text in terms of cognitive load, or the amount of information actively maintained in one's mind (also see working memory).[51] One study showed that going from about 5 hyperlinks per page to about 11 per page reduced college students' understanding (assessed by multiple choice tests) of articles about alternative energy.[52] This can be attributed to the decision-making process (deciding whether to click on it) required by each hyperlink,[51] which may reduce comprehension of surrounding text.


On the other hand, other studies have shown that if a short summary of the link's content is provided when the mouse pointer hovers over it, then comprehension of the text is improved.[53] "Navigation hints" about which links are most relevant improved comprehension.[54] Finally, the background knowledge of the reader can partially determine the effect hyperlinks have on comprehension. In a study of reading comprehension with subjects who were familiar or unfamiliar with art history, texts which were hyperlinked to one another hierarchically were easier for novices to understand than texts which were hyperlinked semantically. In contrast, those already familiar with the topic understood the content equally well with both types of organization.[51]


In interpreting these results, it may be useful to note that the studies mentioned were all performed in closed content environments, not on the internet. That is, the texts used only linked to a predetermined set of other texts which was offline. Furthermore, the participants were explicitly instructed to read on a certain topic in a limited amount of time. Reading text on the internet may not have these constraints.

Professional development[edit]

The National Reading Panel noted that comprehension strategy instruction is difficult for many teachers as well as for students, particularly because they were not taught this way and because it is a demanding task. They suggested that professional development can increase teachers/students willingness to use reading strategies but admitted that much remains to be done in this area.


The directed listening and thinking activity is a technique available to teachers to aid students in learning how to un-read and reading comprehension. It is also difficult for students that are new. There is often some debate when considering the relationship between reading fluency and reading comprehension. There is evidence of a direct correlation that fluency and comprehension lead to better understanding of the written material, across all ages.[55] The National Assessment of Educational Progress assessed U.S. student performance in reading at grade 12 from both public and private school population and found that only 37 percent of students had proficient skills. The majority, 72 percent of the students, were only at or above basic skills, and 28 percent of the students were below basic level.[56]

Tompkins, Gail E. (2011). Literacy in the Early Grades: A Successful Start for Prek-4 Readers and Writers. Allyn and Bacon.  978-0-13-702787-3.

ISBN

Heim S, Friederici AD (November 2003). "Phonological processing in language production: time course of brain activity". NeuroReport. 14 (16): 2031–3. :10.1097/00001756-200311140-00005. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0010-D0B5-7. PMID 14600492.

doi

Vigneau M, Beaucousin V, Hervé PY, et al. (May 2006). "Meta-analyzing left hemisphere language areas: phonology, semantics, and sentence processing". NeuroImage. 30 (4): 1414–32. :10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.002. PMID 16413796. S2CID 8870165.

doi

Express English Language Training Center

Info, Tips, and Strategies for PTE Read Aloud

Andrews University

English Reading Comprehension Skills

ProductiveFish

SQ3R Reading Strategy And How to Apply It

– From the ERIC Clearinghouse on Reading English and Communication.

Vocabulary Instruction and Reading comprehension

ReadWorks.org | The Solution to Reading Comprehension