As with any logical fallacy, identifying that the reasoning behind an argument is flawed does not necessarily imply that the resulting conclusion is false. Statistical methods have been proposed that use correlation as the basis for hypothesis tests for causality, including the Granger causality test and convergent cross mapping. The Bradford Hill criteria, also known as Hill's criteria for causation, are a group of nine principles that can be useful in establishing epidemiologic evidence of a causal relationship.

Usage and meaning of terms[edit]

"Imply"[edit]

In casual use, the word "implies" loosely means suggests, rather than requires. However, in logic, the technical use of the word "implies" means "is a sufficient condition for."[3] That is the meaning intended by statisticians when they say causation is not certain. Indeed, p implies q has the technical meaning of the material conditional: if p then q symbolized as p → q. That is, "if circumstance p is true, then q follows." In that sense, it is always correct to say "Correlation does not imply causation."

"Cause"[edit]

The word "cause" (or "causation") has multiple meanings in English. In philosophical terminology, "cause" can refer to necessary, sufficient, or contributing causes. In examining correlation, "cause" is most often used to mean "one contributing cause" (but not necessarily the only contributing cause).

Examples of illogically inferring causation from correlation[edit]

B causes A (reverse causation or reverse causality)[edit]

Reverse causation or reverse causality or wrong direction is an informal fallacy of questionable cause where cause and effect are reversed. The cause is said to be the effect and vice versa.

Use of correlation as scientific evidence[edit]

Much of scientific evidence is based upon a correlation of variables[19] that are observed to occur together. Scientists are careful to point out that correlation does not necessarily mean causation. The assumption that A causes B simply because A correlates with B is often not accepted as a legitimate form of argument.


However, sometimes people commit the opposite fallacy of dismissing correlation entirely. That would dismiss a large swath of important scientific evidence.[19] Since it may be difficult or ethically impossible to run controlled double-blind studies, correlational evidence from several different angles may be useful for prediction despite failing to provide evidence for causation. For example, social workers might be interested in knowing how child abuse relates to academic performance. Although it would be unethical to perform an experiment in which children are randomly assigned to receive or not receive abuse, researchers can look at existing groups using a non-experimental correlational design. If in fact a negative correlation exists between abuse and academic performance, researchers could potentially use this knowledge of a statistical correlation to make predictions about children outside the study who experience abuse even though the study failed to provide causal evidence that abuse decreases academic performance.[20] The combination of limited available methodologies with the dismissing correlation fallacy has on occasion been used to counter a scientific finding. For example, the tobacco industry has historically relied on a dismissal of correlational evidence to reject a link between tobacco smoke and lung cancer,[21] as did biologist and statistician Ronald Fisher (frequently on the industry's behalf).[list 1]


Correlation is a valuable type of scientific evidence in fields such as medicine, psychology, and sociology. Correlations must first be confirmed as real, and every possible causative relationship must then be systematically explored. In the end, correlation alone cannot be used as evidence for a cause-and-effect relationship between a treatment and benefit, a risk factor and a disease, or a social or economic factor and various outcomes. It is one of the most abused types of evidence because it is easy and even tempting to come to premature conclusions based upon the preliminary appearance of a correlation.[21]

 – Type of fallacious argument (logical fallacy)

Affirming the consequent

 – Phenomenon in statistics

Alignments of random points

 – Evidence relying on personal testimony

Anecdotal evidence

Apophenia

Post hoc analysis

 – Purported set of secret messages encoded within the Hebrew text of the Torah

Bible code

Bradford Hill criteria

 – Concurrence of events with no connection

Coincidence#Causality

 – Variable or factor in causal inference

Confounding

 – Logical fallacy

Confusion of the inverse

 – Observation that amount heart diseases French people have is much less than is expected

French paradox

 – Design of tasks

Design of experiments

 – Apparent, but false, correlation between causally-independent variables

Joint effect

 – Statistical model

Mediation (statistics)

Normally distributed and uncorrelated does not imply independent

 – Satirical deity

Pirates and global warming

 – Aspect of scientific research

Reproducibility

 – Apparent, but false, correlation between causally-independent variables

Spurious relationship

 – Thinking in terms of destiny or purpose

Teleology

Beebee, Helen; Hitchcock, Christopher; Menzies, Peter (2009). . Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-162946-4.

The Oxford Handbook of Causation