Katana VentraIP

Cognitive neuroscience

Cognitive neuroscience is the scientific field that is concerned with the study of the biological processes and aspects that underlie cognition,[1] with a specific focus on the neural connections in the brain which are involved in mental processes. It addresses the questions of how cognitive activities are affected or controlled by neural circuits in the brain. Cognitive neuroscience is a branch of both neuroscience and psychology, overlapping with disciplines such as behavioral neuroscience, cognitive psychology, physiological psychology and affective neuroscience.[2] Cognitive neuroscience relies upon theories in cognitive science coupled with evidence from neurobiology, and computational modeling.[2]

For the academic journal, see Cognitive Neuroscience.

Parts of the brain play an important role in this field. Neurons play the most vital role, since the main point is to establish an understanding of cognition from a neural perspective, along with the different lobes of the cerebral cortex.


Methods employed in cognitive neuroscience include experimental procedures from psychophysics and cognitive psychology, functional neuroimaging, electrophysiology, cognitive genomics, and behavioral genetics.


Studies of patients with cognitive deficits due to brain lesions constitute an important aspect of cognitive neuroscience. The damages in lesioned brains provide a comparable starting point on regards to healthy and fully functioning brains. These damages change the neural circuits in the brain and cause it to malfunction during basic cognitive processes, such as memory or learning. People have learning disabilities and such damage, can be compared with how the healthy neural circuits are functioning, and possibly draw conclusions about the basis of the affected cognitive processes. Some examples of learning disabilities in the brain include places in Wernicke's area, the left side of the temporal lobe, and Broca's area close to the frontal lobe.[3]


Also, cognitive abilities based on brain development are studied and examined under the subfield of developmental cognitive neuroscience. This shows brain development over time, analyzing differences and concocting possible reasons for those differences.


Theoretical approaches include computational neuroscience and cognitive psychology.

Emergence of a new discipline[edit]

Birth of cognitive science[edit]

On September 11, 1956, a large-scale meeting of cognitivists took place at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology. George A. Miller presented his "The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two" paper[23] while Noam Chomsky and Newell & Simon presented their findings on computer science. Ulric Neisser commented on many of the findings at this meeting in his 1967 book Cognitive Psychology. The term "psychology" had been waning in the 1950s and 1960s, causing the field to be referred to as "cognitive science". Behaviorists such as Miller began to focus on the representation of language rather than general behavior. David Marr concluded that one should understand any cognitive process at three levels of analysis. These levels include computational, algorithmic/representational, and physical levels of analysis.[24]

Combining neuroscience and cognitive science[edit]

Before the 1980s, interaction between neuroscience and cognitive science was scarce.[25] Cognitive neuroscience began to integrate the newly laid theoretical ground in cognitive science, that emerged between the 1950s and 1960s, with approaches in experimental psychology, neuropsychology and neuroscience. (Neuroscience was not established as a unified discipline until 1971[26]). In the late 1970s, neuroscientist Michael S. Gazzaniga and cognitive psychologist George A. Miller were said to have first coined the term "cognitive neuroscience."[27] In the very late 20th century new technologies evolved that are now the mainstay of the methodology of cognitive neuroscience, including TMS (1985) and fMRI (1991). Earlier methods used in cognitive neuroscience include EEG (human EEG 1920) and MEG (1968). Occasionally cognitive neuroscientists utilize other brain imaging methods such as PET and SPECT. An upcoming technique in neuroscience is NIRS which uses light absorption to calculate changes in oxy- and deoxyhemoglobin in cortical areas. In some animals Single-unit recording can be used. Other methods include microneurography, facial EMG, and eye tracking. Integrative neuroscience attempts to consolidate data in databases, and form unified descriptive models from various fields and scales: biology, psychology, anatomy, and clinical practice.[28]

Adaptive resonance theory (ART) is a cognitive neuroscience theory developed by Gail Carpenter and Stephen Grossberg in the late 1970s on aspects of how the brain processes information. It describes a number of artificial neural network models which use supervised and unsupervised learning methods, and address problems such as pattern recognition and prediction.[29]


In 2014, Stanislas Dehaene, Giacomo Rizzolatti and Trevor Robbins, were awarded the Brain Prize "for their pioneering research on higher brain mechanisms underpinning such complex human functions as literacy, numeracy, motivated behaviour and social cognition, and for their efforts to understand cognitive and behavioural disorders".[30] Brenda Milner, Marcus Raichle and John O'Keefe received the Kavli Prize in Neuroscience "for the discovery of specialized brain networks for memory and cognition"[31] and O'Keefe shared the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine in the same year with May-Britt Moser and Edvard Moser "for their discoveries of cells that constitute a positioning system in the brain".[32]


In 2017, Wolfram Schultz, Peter Dayan and Ray Dolan were awarded the Brain Prize "for their multidisciplinary analysis of brain mechanisms that link learning to reward, which has far-reaching implications for the understanding of human behaviour, including disorders of decision-making in conditions such as gambling, drug addiction, compulsive behaviour and schizophrenia".,[33]

Recent trends[edit]

Recently the focus of research had expanded from the localization of brain area(s) for specific functions in the adult brain using a single technology. Studies have been diverging in several different directions: exploring the interactions between different brain areas, using multiple technologies and approaches to understand brain functions, and using computational approaches.[34] Advances in non-invasive functional neuroimaging and associated data analysis methods have also made it possible to use highly naturalistic stimuli and tasks such as feature films depicting social interactions in cognitive neuroscience studies.[35]


Another very recent trend in cognitive neuroscience is the use of optogenetics to explore circuit function and its behavioral consequences.[36]

Attention

Cognitive development

Consciousness

Creativity

Decision-making

Emotions

Intelligence

Language

Learning

Memory

Perception

Social cognition

Mind Wandering

Psychophysics

Eye-tracking

Functional magnetic resonance imaging

Electroencephalography

Magnetoencephalography

Electrocorticography

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

Computational Modeling

Experimental methods include:

Danish neuroscientist and former university professor

Jesper Mogensen

Baars, Bernard J.; Gage, Nicole M. (2010). Cognition, Brain, and Consciousness: Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience. Academic Press.  978-0-12-381440-1.

ISBN

Bear, Mark F.; Connors, Barry W.; Paradiso, Michael A. (2007). Neuroscience. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.  978-0-7817-6003-4.

ISBN

; Sejnowski, Terrence Joseph (1992). The Computational Brain. MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-33965-0.

Churchland, Patricia Smith

Code, Chris (2004). "Classic Cases: Ancient and Modern Milestones in the Development of Neuropsychological Science". In Code, Chris; Joanette, Yves; Lecours, André Roch; Wallesch, Claus-W (eds.). Classic Cases in Neuropsychology. pp. 17–25. :10.4324/9780203304112-8. ISBN 978-0-203-30411-2.

doi

Enersen, O. D. (2009). John Hughlings Jackson. In: Who Named It. Retrieved 14 August 2009

http://www.whonamedit.com/doctor.cfm/2766.html

Ivry, R. B. & Mangun, G. R. (2002). Cognitive Neuroscience: The biology of the mind (2nd ed.). New York: W.W.Norton.

Gazzaniga, M. S.

Gallistel, R. (2009). "Memory and the Computational Brain: Why Cognitive Science will Transform Neuroscience." ISBN 978-1-4051-2287-0.

Wiley-Blackwell

The Cognitive Neurosciences III, (2004), The MIT Press, ISBN 0-262-07254-8

Gazzaniga, M. S.

Ed. (1999). Conversations in the Cognitive Neurosciences, The MIT Press, ISBN 0-262-57117-X.

Gazzaniga, M. S.

Sternberg, Eliezer J. Are You a Machine? The Brain, the Mind and What it Means to be Human. Amherst, NY: Prometheus Books.

Ward, Jamie (2015). (3rd ed.). Psychology Press. ISBN 978-1848722729.

The Student's Guide to Cognitive Neuroscience

Handbook of Functional Neuroimaging of Cognition By Roberto Cabeza, Alan Kingstone

Principles of neural science By Eric R. Kandel, James H. Schwartz, Thomas M. Jessell

The Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory By Amanda Parker, Edward L. Wilding, Timothy J. Bussey

Neuronal Theories of the Brain By Christof Koch, Joel L. Davis

Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning By Keith James Holyoak, Robert G. Morrison

Handbook of Mathematical Cognition By Jamie I. D. Campbell

Cognitive Psychology By Michael W. Eysenck, Mark T. Keane

Development of Intelligence By Mike Anderson

Development of Mental Processing By Andreas Demetriou, et al.

Memory and Thinking By Robert H. Logie, K. J. Gilhooly

Memory Capacity By Nelson Cowan

Proceedings of the Nineteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science

Models of Working Memory By Akira Miyake, Priti Shah

Memory and Thinking By Robert H. Logie, K. J. Gilhooly

Variation in Working Memory By Andrew R. A. Conway, et al.

Memory Capacity By Nelson Cowan

Cognition and Intelligence By Robert J. Sternberg, Jean E. Pretz

General Factor of Intelligence By Robert J. Sternberg, Elena Grigorenko

Neurological Basis of Learning, Development and Discovery By Anton E. Lawson

Memory and Human Cognition By John T. E. Richardson

Society for Neuroscience. Retrieved 14 August 2009

https://web.archive.org/web/20090805111859/http://www.sfn.org/index.cfm?pagename=about_SfN#timeline

,"Current Opinion in Neurobiology", (2007)

Keiji Tanaka

Cognitive Neuroscience Society Homepage

There's Something about Zero

What Is Cognitive Neuroscience?, Jamie Ward/Psychology Press

goCognitive - Educational Tools for Cognitive Neuroscience (including video interviews)

CogNet, The Brain and Cognitive Sciences Community Online, MIT

Cognitive Neuroscience Arena, Psychology Press

Cognitive Neuroscience and Philosophy, CUJCS, Spring 2002

Whole Brain Atlas Top 100 Brain Structures

Cognitive Neuroscience Discussion Group

John Jonides, a big role in Cognitive Neurosciences by Beebrite

Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience

AgliotiLAB - Social and Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory founded in 2003 in Rome, Italy

Related Wikibooks